Page 129 - Costellazioni 5
P. 129

MARCO CARACCIOLO, A Walk Through Deep History


                statement, without any overt reference to the body: “Your relationship
                was very important to you.” The study found that participants in the
                metaphorical condition tended to walk for a longer time, and ended up
                farther away from the start position, than those in the nonmetaphorical
                condition. Gibbs reads this result as confirmation for his embodied sim-
                ulation theory of metaphor understanding: we understand embodied
                language by enacting, internally and mostly unconsciously, the verbally
                portrayed embodied action; in the case of Gibbs’s experiment, this sim-
                ulation mechanism primed the participants towards bodily movement
                and subtly boosted their performance when it came to actual walking.
                     This is just a simple example of how language understanding trig-
                gers embodied responses, which can have very overt effects on how peo-
                ple use their bodies. In other scenarios, the role of the body in language
                comprehension remains covert—unless scientists probe it by way of brain
                imaging or more indirect, behavioral methods (for instance, by measur-
                ing response times to linguistic stimuli). Friedemann Pulvermüller has
                reviewed neuroscientific studies linking language comprehension to the
                                                              7
                motor cortex, including so-called mirror neurons. We understand verbs
                denoting embodied actions by relying on some of the same neural struc-
                tures that are implicated in performing real actions. Consider verbal
                phrases such as ‘holding on’ and ‘le ing go’ in the Salopek passage dis-
                cussed above: processing these expressions activates neural mechanisms
                involved in the physical gesture of holding onto an object or le ing go of
                it. Behavioral research in psycholinguistics—for example, by Arthur
                Glenberg and Michael Kaschak, or Martin Fischer and Rolf Zwaan—con-
                firms this intuition: after parsing a sentence suggesting forward motion
                (such as Salopek’s “Walking is falling forward”), readers will be quicker
                                                                    8
                to push a lever forward than in the opposite direction. Put otherwise,




                7
                 Friedemann Pulvermüller, “Brain Mechanisms Linking Language and Ac-
                tion,” Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 6, no. 7 (2005): 576-82; for an analogous
                argument in the domain of conceptualization, see also Vi orio Gallese and
                George Lakoff, “The Brain’s Concepts: The Role of the Sensory-Motor System
                in Conceptual Knowledge,” Cognitive Neuropsychology, no. 22 (2005): 455-79.
                8  Arthur M. Glenberg and Michael P. Kaschak, “Grounding Language in Ac-
                tion,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9, no. 3 (2002): 558-65; Martin H. Fischer


                                                128
   124   125   126   127   128   129   130   131   132   133   134