Page 65 - Costellazioni 5
P. 65

PATRICK COLM HOGAN, Affective Space and Emotional Time

                so to speak absolute “up” (both egocentric and allocentric), is emo-
                tional as well. The lines may be taken to hint that the tiāndào or norm
                of life is contrary to the spatial isolation of the lovers—thus contrary
                to the husband’s departure. The suggestion would seem to be that,
                if he had followed the dào, he would have stayed with his mate, like
                the paired butterflies in the west garden, or like the legendary youth
                who prized waiting for his beloved even more than his own life. This
                usage is in part enabled by the fact that the dào itself is a spatial
                metaphor organizing one’s proper relation to society in terms of one’s
                relation to a trajectory of movement in space.
                     A key feature of the neuroscience of space is the division
                among “bodily space, peripersonal space, and extrapersonal space,”
                which is to say, quoting De Vignemont and Iannetti, the space of
                “sensory stimuli happening on the body, […] the space immediately
                surrounding the body” and “the space beyond reach” (327; the au-
                thors are drawing on seminal research by Rizzolatti et al.). Cléry and
                colleagues explain that, “While space is perceived as unitary, exper-
                imental evidence indicates that the brain actually contains a modular
                representation of space, specific cortical regions being involved in
                the processing of extra-personal space, that is the space that is far
                away from the subject and that cannot be directly acted upon by the
                body, while other cortical regions process peripersonal space, that is
                the space that directly surrounds us and which we can act upon”
                (313, also drawing on the work of Rizzolatti et al.). To a certain extent,
                writers have recognized the relation between this spatial division and
                emotion. Thus, Ferri and colleagues, referring to peripersonal space,
                note that “We live in a dynamic environment, constantly confronted
                with approaching objects that we may either avoid or be forced to
                address” (16328). More explicitly, Cléry and colleagues, stress the im-
                portance of “defense and obstacle avoidance behavior” and “protec-
                tion of near peripersonal space or safety margin around the body”
                (319, drawing on work by Graziano and colleagues along with other
                research). But the treatment of the relation between emotion and
                forms of spatial experience has been quite limited, at least in much
                of the research.
                     Before we consider these forms of spatiality further, it is valu-
                able to broach one further topic in the neuroscience of space, bound-
                aries. Grieves and Jeffery point out that a “widely researched, but
                often overlooked cell type responds purely to environmental bound-
                aries” (120; see also Groh 84 on “border ownership”). Lǐ Bái’s poem



                                                64
   60   61   62   63   64   65   66   67   68   69   70